Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice

David R. Feinberg, Benedict C. Jones, Miriam J. Law Smith, Fhionna R. Moore, Lisa M. DeBruine, R. Elisabeth Cornwell, S. G. Hillier, David I. Perrett

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  • 181 Citations

Abstract

Men with low testosterone (feminine men) invest in relationships and offspring more than men with high testosterone (masculine men). Women’s attraction to testosterone dependent traits (e.g. masculine face shape) is enhanced during the late-follicular, fertile phase of the menstrual cycle. Attractive, feminine women have stronger preferences for masculine men as possible long-term partners than less attractive, masculine women. We manipulated 2 testosterone related vocal traits (voice pitch and apparent vocal-tract length) in voices to test if women prefer masculinized men’s voices to feminized men’s voices; masculinity preferences are enhanced at the fertile (late-follicular) menstrual cycle phase; the amount that masculinity preferences shift cyclically relates to average estrone-3-glucuronide concentration (the primary urinary metabolite of estrone, E3G). We found women displayed general masculinity preferences for men’s voices; masculinity preferences were greater in the fertile (late-follicular) phase of the cycle than the non-fertile (early-follicular and luteal) phase; and this effect was most pronounced for women with low average E3G concentration. As feminine women (i.e. those with high average E3G levels) are most able to obtain investment even from masculine men, these women may not need to change their mating preference or strategy during the menstrual cycle as much as masculine women.
Original languageEnglish
Pages (from-to)215-222
Number of pages8
JournalHormones and Behavior
Volume49
Issue number2
DOIs
StatePublished - Feb 2006

Fingerprint

Masculinity
Menstrual Cycle
Estrogens
Follicular Phase
Testosterone
Estrone
Luteal Phase
Glucuronides

Cite this

Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., Law Smith, M. J., Moore, F. R., DeBruine, L. M., Cornwell, R. E., ... Perrett, D. I. (2006). Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice. Hormones and Behavior, 49(2), 215-222. DOI: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.004

Feinberg, David R.; Jones, Benedict C.; Law Smith, Miriam J.; Moore, Fhionna R.; DeBruine, Lisa M.; Cornwell, R. Elisabeth; Hillier, S. G.; Perrett, David I. / Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice.

In: Hormones and Behavior, Vol. 49, No. 2, 02.2006, p. 215-222.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

@article{942a129859f9438d8c0b7516790cbaaf,
title = "Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice",
abstract = "Men with low testosterone (feminine men) invest in relationships and offspring more than men with high testosterone (masculine men). Women’s attraction to testosterone dependent traits (e.g. masculine face shape) is enhanced during the late-follicular, fertile phase of the menstrual cycle. Attractive, feminine women have stronger preferences for masculine men as possible long-term partners than less attractive, masculine women. We manipulated 2 testosterone related vocal traits (voice pitch and apparent vocal-tract length) in voices to test if women prefer masculinized men’s voices to feminized men’s voices; masculinity preferences are enhanced at the fertile (late-follicular) menstrual cycle phase; the amount that masculinity preferences shift cyclically relates to average estrone-3-glucuronide concentration (the primary urinary metabolite of estrone, E3G). We found women displayed general masculinity preferences for men’s voices; masculinity preferences were greater in the fertile (late-follicular) phase of the cycle than the non-fertile (early-follicular and luteal) phase; and this effect was most pronounced for women with low average E3G concentration. As feminine women (i.e. those with high average E3G levels) are most able to obtain investment even from masculine men, these women may not need to change their mating preference or strategy during the menstrual cycle as much as masculine women.",
author = "Feinberg, {David R.} and Jones, {Benedict C.} and {Law Smith}, {Miriam J.} and Moore, {Fhionna R.} and DeBruine, {Lisa M.} and Cornwell, {R. Elisabeth} and Hillier, {S. G.} and Perrett, {David I.}",
year = "2006",
month = "2",
doi = "10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.004",
volume = "49",
pages = "215--222",
journal = "Hormones and Behavior",
issn = "0018-506X",
publisher = "Academic Press Inc.",
number = "2",

}

Feinberg, DR, Jones, BC, Law Smith, MJ, Moore, FR, DeBruine, LM, Cornwell, RE, Hillier, SG & Perrett, DI 2006, 'Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice' Hormones and Behavior, vol 49, no. 2, pp. 215-222. DOI: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.004

Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice. / Feinberg, David R.; Jones, Benedict C.; Law Smith, Miriam J.; Moore, Fhionna R.; DeBruine, Lisa M.; Cornwell, R. Elisabeth; Hillier, S. G.; Perrett, David I.

In: Hormones and Behavior, Vol. 49, No. 2, 02.2006, p. 215-222.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

TY - JOUR

T1 - Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice

AU - Feinberg,David R.

AU - Jones,Benedict C.

AU - Law Smith,Miriam J.

AU - Moore,Fhionna R.

AU - DeBruine,Lisa M.

AU - Cornwell,R. Elisabeth

AU - Hillier,S. G.

AU - Perrett,David I.

PY - 2006/2

Y1 - 2006/2

N2 - Men with low testosterone (feminine men) invest in relationships and offspring more than men with high testosterone (masculine men). Women’s attraction to testosterone dependent traits (e.g. masculine face shape) is enhanced during the late-follicular, fertile phase of the menstrual cycle. Attractive, feminine women have stronger preferences for masculine men as possible long-term partners than less attractive, masculine women. We manipulated 2 testosterone related vocal traits (voice pitch and apparent vocal-tract length) in voices to test if women prefer masculinized men’s voices to feminized men’s voices; masculinity preferences are enhanced at the fertile (late-follicular) menstrual cycle phase; the amount that masculinity preferences shift cyclically relates to average estrone-3-glucuronide concentration (the primary urinary metabolite of estrone, E3G). We found women displayed general masculinity preferences for men’s voices; masculinity preferences were greater in the fertile (late-follicular) phase of the cycle than the non-fertile (early-follicular and luteal) phase; and this effect was most pronounced for women with low average E3G concentration. As feminine women (i.e. those with high average E3G levels) are most able to obtain investment even from masculine men, these women may not need to change their mating preference or strategy during the menstrual cycle as much as masculine women.

AB - Men with low testosterone (feminine men) invest in relationships and offspring more than men with high testosterone (masculine men). Women’s attraction to testosterone dependent traits (e.g. masculine face shape) is enhanced during the late-follicular, fertile phase of the menstrual cycle. Attractive, feminine women have stronger preferences for masculine men as possible long-term partners than less attractive, masculine women. We manipulated 2 testosterone related vocal traits (voice pitch and apparent vocal-tract length) in voices to test if women prefer masculinized men’s voices to feminized men’s voices; masculinity preferences are enhanced at the fertile (late-follicular) menstrual cycle phase; the amount that masculinity preferences shift cyclically relates to average estrone-3-glucuronide concentration (the primary urinary metabolite of estrone, E3G). We found women displayed general masculinity preferences for men’s voices; masculinity preferences were greater in the fertile (late-follicular) phase of the cycle than the non-fertile (early-follicular and luteal) phase; and this effect was most pronounced for women with low average E3G concentration. As feminine women (i.e. those with high average E3G levels) are most able to obtain investment even from masculine men, these women may not need to change their mating preference or strategy during the menstrual cycle as much as masculine women.

U2 - 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.004

DO - 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.004

M3 - Article

VL - 49

SP - 215

EP - 222

JO - Hormones and Behavior

T2 - Hormones and Behavior

JF - Hormones and Behavior

SN - 0018-506X

IS - 2

ER -

Feinberg DR, Jones BC, Law Smith MJ, Moore FR, DeBruine LM, Cornwell RE et al. Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice. Hormones and Behavior. 2006 Feb;49(2):215-222. Available from, DOI: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.004